[1]
Siegler, Robert S. and Alibali, Martha Wagner, Children’s thinking, 4th ed., Intl. ed. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson/Prentice Hall, 2005.
[2]
Goswami, Usha C., Cognitive development: the learning brain. Hove: Psychology Press, 2008.
[3]
R. D. Parke and A. Clarke-Stewart, Social development, Second edition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 2014.
[4]
Schaffer, H. Rudolph, Social development. Oxford: Blackwell, 1996.
[5]
Siegler, Robert S., DeLoache, Judy S., and Eisenberg, Nancy, How children develop, 3rd ed., International ed. New York: Worth, 2011.
[6]
Ginsburg, Herbert, Opper, Sylvia, ‘Biography and Basic Ideas’, in Piaget’s theory of intellectual development, London: Prentice Hall International, 1988, pp. 1–25.
[7]
Bremner, J. Gavin, Infancy, 2nd ed. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1994.
[8]
Bremner, J. Gavin and Fogel, Alan, Blackwell handbook of infant development, vol. Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley InterScience, 2007 [Online]. Available: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470996348
[9]
Bremner, J. Gavin and Wachs, Theodore D., The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of infant development, 2nd ed., vol. Wiley-Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, 2010.
[10]
Renée Baillargeon, ‘The Acquisition of Physical Knowledge in Infancy: A Summary in Eight Lessons’, in Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley InterScience, 2007, pp. 47–83 [Online]. Available: https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch3
[11]
R, Baillargeon, ‘The Acquisition of Physical Knowledge in Infancy: A Summary in Eight Lessons’, in The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, 2nd ed., vol. Wiley-Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology, Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2011, pp. 47–83 [Online]. Available: https://ucl.primo.exlibrisgroup.com/view/action/uresolver.do?operation=resolveService&package_service_id=14387402700004761&institutionId=4761&customerId=4760&VE=true
[12]
Jean M. Mandler, ‘A New Perspective on Cognitive Development in Infancy’, American Scientist, vol. 78, no. 3, pp. 236–243 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/29774030
[13]
J. M. Mandler, ‘On the Spatial Foundations of the Conceptual System and Its Enrichment’, Cognitive Science, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 421–451, 2012, doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01241.x.
[14]
Spelke, Elizabeth S., ‘Core Knowledge’, American Psychologist, vol. 55, no. 11, pp. 1233–1243 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&CSC=Y&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&AN=00000487-200011000-00009&LSLINK=80&D=ovft
[15]
Hood, Bruce, ‘Looking and Search Measures of Object Knowledge in Preschool Children’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 61–70 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&CSC=Y&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&AN=00063061-200301000-00005&LSLINK=80&D=ovft
[16]
S. T. Baker, N. L. Gjersoe, K. Sibielska-Woch, A. M. Leslie, and B. M. Hood, ‘Inhibitory control interacts with core knowledge in toddlers’ manual search for an occluded object’, Developmental Science, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 270–279, Mar. 2011, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00972.x.
[17]
Berk, Laura E., ‘Language Development’, in Child development, 9th ed., International ed., Boston [Mass.]: Pearson, 2013, pp. 358–399.
[18]
Gleitman, Henry, et al, ‘Language’, in Psychology, 7th ed., New York: W. W. Norton & Co, 2007, pp. 378–423.
[19]
E. V. Clark, ‘How language acquisition builds on cognitive development’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 8, no. 10, pp. 472–478, Oct. 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.012.
[20]
E. L. Newport, ‘Maturational constraints on language learning’, Cognitive Science, doi: 10.1016/0364-0213(90)90024-Q.
[21]
J. R. Saffran, ‘Statistical language learning: mechanisms and constraints’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 110–114, Aug. 2003, doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01243.
[22]
A. Senghas and M. Coppola, ‘Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar’, Psychological Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 323–328, Jul. 2001, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00359.
[23]
A. Senghas and M. Coppola, ‘Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar’, Psychological Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 323–328, Jul. 2001, doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00359.
[24]
M. Tomasello, ‘Do young children have adult syntactic competence?’, Cognition, vol. 74, no. 3, pp. 209–253, Mar. 2000, doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00069-4.
[25]
O. Houdé et al., ‘Functional magnetic resonance imaging study of Piaget’s conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children: A neo-Piagetian approach’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 110, no. 3, pp. 332–346, Nov. 2011, doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.008.
[26]
‘British Journal of Psychology’, vol. 73, no. 2 [Online]. Available: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/20448295/1982/73/2
[27]
D. Eames, D. Shorrocks, and P. Tomlinson, ‘Naughty animals or naughty experimenters? Conservation accidents revisited with video-stimulated commentary’, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 25–37, Mar. 1990, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1990.tb00819.x.
[28]
R. Siegler, ‘How Does Change Occur: A Microgenetic Study of Number Conservation’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 225–273, 1995, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1006.
[29]
K. Wynn, ‘Children’s understanding of counting’, Cognition, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 155–193, 1990, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3.
[30]
L. Feigenson, S. Dehaene, and E. Spelke, ‘Core systems of number’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 8, no. 7, pp. 307–314, 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002.
[31]
R. Siegler, ‘How Does Change Occur: A Microgenetic Study of Number Conservation’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 225–273, Jun. 1995, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1006.
[32]
M. Le Corre and S. Carey, ‘One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles’, Cognition, vol. 105, no. 2, pp. 395–438, Nov. 2007, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005.
[33]
Hughes, Martin, Children and number difficulties in learning mathematics. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1986.
[34]
Hughes, Martin, Children and number difficulties in learning mathematics. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1986.
[35]
Fuson, Karen C., Children’s counting and concepts of number, vol. Springer series in cognitive development. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1988.
[36]
Mix, Kelly S., ‘Multiple Cues for Quantification in Infancy: Is Number One of Them?’, Psychological Bulletin, vol. 128, no. 2, pp. 278–294 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&CSC=Y&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&AN=00006823-200203000-00005&LSLINK=80&D=ovft
[37]
F. Xu, E. S. Spelke, and S. Goddard, ‘Number sense in human infants’, Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 88–101, Jan. 2005, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x.
[38]
J. Halberda, M. M. M. Mazzocco, and L. Feigenson, ‘Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement’, Nature, vol. 455, no. 7213, pp. 665–668, 2008, doi: 10.1038/nature07246.
[39]
Medin, Douglas L.1,2, ‘Concepts and Conceptual Structure’, American Psychologist, vol. 44, no. 12, pp. 1469–1481 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-3.12.0b/ovidweb.cgi?QS2=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
[40]
S. Gelman, ‘Psychological essentialism in children’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 8, no. 9, pp. 404–409, Sep. 2004, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.001.
[41]
P. C. Quinn, ‘Early Categorization: A New Synthesis’, in Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, U. Goswami, Ed. Malden, MA, USA: Blackwell Publishers Ltd, 2002, pp. 84–101 [Online]. Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9780470996652.ch4
[42]
S. A. Gelman, ‘Learning from Others: Children’s Construction of Concepts’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 60, no. 1, pp. 115–140, Jan. 2009, doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659.
[43]
J. Huttenlocher and P. Smiley, ‘Early word meanings: The case of object names’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 63–89, 1987, doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90004-1.
[44]
S. S. Smiley and A. L. Brown, ‘Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 249–257, Oct. 1979, doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(79)90087-0.
[45]
E. Rosch, C. B. Mervis, W. D. Gray, D. M. Johnson, and P. Boyes-Braem, ‘Basic objects in natural categories’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 382–439, 1976, doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-X.
[46]
Carolyn. B. Mervis, ‘Child-basic object categories and early lexical development’, in Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization, vol. Emory symposia in cognition, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987, pp. 201–233 [Online]. Available: https://contentstore.cla.co.uk/secure/link?id=9b632cde-a4ae-ec11-a99b-0050f2f06092
[47]
S. A. Gelman and E. M. Markman, ‘Categories and induction in young children’, Cognition, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 183–209, 1986, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X.
[48]
M. J. Farrar, G. E. Raney, and M. E. Boyer, ‘Knowledge, Concepts, and Inferences in Childhood’, Child Development, vol. 63, no. 3, pp. 673–691, Jun. 1992, doi: 10.2307/1131354.
[49]
V. K. Jaswal and L. A. Neely, ‘Adults Don’t Always Know Best: Preschoolers Use Past Reliability Over Age When Learning New Words’, Psychological Science, vol. 17, no. 9, pp. 757–758, Sep. 2006, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x.
[50]
V. K. Jaswal and E. M. Markman, ‘Looks Aren’t Everything: 24-Month-Olds’ Willingness to Accept Unexpected Labels’, Journal of Cognition and Development, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 93–111, Feb. 2007, doi: 10.1080/15248370709336995.
[51]
Ross. A. Thompson and Miranda Goodman, ‘The Architecture of Social Development Science: Theoretical and Historical Persepctives’, in Social development: relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, New York: Guilford Press, 2011, pp. 3–28.
[52]
Oatley, Keith, Keltner, Dacher, and Jenkins, Jennifer M., Understanding emotions, 2nd ed. Malden, MA: Blackwell Pub, 2006.
[53]
L. A. Sroufe, ‘Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood’, Attachment & Human Development, vol. 7, no. 4, pp. 349–367, Dec. 2005, doi: 10.1080/14616730500365928.
[54]
H. F. Harlow and S. J. Suomi, ‘Nature of love: Simplified.’, American Psychologist, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 161–168, 1970 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-3.33.0b/ovidweb.cgi?QS2=434f4e1a73d37e8c3c09c5e652bc6aa0a22d7b023d33a19afdf14a26c89e056cda011a00de4155c9d8f1e6cadb85838d8120ca712f9632ce1816b0f7107df5f3a1b995d01bdbcbb6914504c88cc3eb26a9ee8c62b2fe7c4947eafc81755ef20ac18d985dcd3876331195c828646097949a22aa1d4f492d923fca48b3108075479fe04b122401a9ff779f5a355f29b01f3889ce0753d65ba08a5fc500966e86488fc4fef92ad5bde483d2eac305b8f0cb7dc01ae44177148b53c520ef6845b43722e46fd168eb986bf5c4f65cc092af9fc257f376f2b172f737f41f7b2e673a63
[55]
George, W. Holden, Brigitte, Vittru & Lisa. H. Rosen, ‘Families Parenting and Discipline’, in Social development: relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, New York: Guilford Press, 2011, pp. 127–152.
[56]
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network, ‘Does Amount of Time Spent in Child Care Predict Socioemotional Adjustment during the Transition to Kindergarten?’, Child Development, vol. 74, no. 4, pp. 976–1005 [Online]. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3696198
[57]
J. Belsky, D. L. Vandell, M. Burchinal, K. A. Clarke-Stewart, K. McCartney, and M. T. Owen, ‘Are There Long-Term Effects of Early Child Care?’, Child Development, vol. 78, no. 2, pp. 681–701, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01021.x.
[58]
L. A. Sroufe, ‘Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood’, Attachment & Human Development, vol. 7, no. 4, pp. 349–367, Dec. 2005, doi: 10.1080/14616730500365928.
[59]
‘American Psychologist’, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 117–208.
[60]
Ross. A. Thompson and Miranda Goodman, ‘The Architecture of Social Development Science: Theoretical and Historical Persepctives’, in Social development: relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, New York: Guilford Press, 2011, pp. 3–28.
[61]
‘UCL Library Services Explore - social development underwood’. [Online]. Available: http://ucl-primo.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/primo_library/libweb/action/search.do?dscnt=0&frbg=&scp.scps=scope%3A%28UCL_LMS_DS%29&tab=local&dstmp=1389366548443&srt=rank&ct=search&mode=Basic&dum=true&indx=1&vl(freeText0)=social%20development%20underwood&fn=search&vid=UCL_VU1
[62]
George, W. Holden, Brigitte, Vittru & Lisa. H. Rosen, ‘Families Parenting and Discipline’, in Social development: relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, New York: Guilford Press, 2011, pp. 127–152.
[63]
H. Harlow and S. Suomi, ‘The nature of love - simplified’, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 117–208 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-3.33.0b/ovidweb.cgi?&S=DHMFFPCCEGDDCFKFNCDKLHDCAAHMAA00&Abstract=S.sh.31%7c1%7c1&Counter5=CRS_abstract%7c00000487-197002000-00006%7cyrovft%7covftdb%7cyrovfta
[64]
H. F. Harlow and S. J. Suomi, ‘Nature of love: Simplified.’, American Psychologist, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 161–168, 1970 [Online]. Available: http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-3.33.0b/ovidweb.cgi?QS2=434f4e1a73d37e8c3c09c5e652bc6aa0a22d7b023d33a19afdf14a26c89e056cda011a00de4155c9d8f1e6cadb85838d8120ca712f9632ce1816b0f7107df5f3a1b995d01bdbcbb6914504c88cc3eb26a9ee8c62b2fe7c4947eafc81755ef20ac18d985dcd3876331195c828646097949a22aa1d4f492d923fca48b3108075479fe04b122401a9ff779f5a355f29b01f3889ce0753d65ba08a5fc500966e86488fc4fef92ad5bde483d2eac305b8f0cb7dc01ae44177148b53c520ef6845b43722e46fd168eb986bf5c4f65cc092af9fc257f376f2b172f737f41f7b2e673a63
[65]
Oatley, Keith, Keltner, Dacher, and Jenkins, Jennifer M., Understanding emotions, 2nd ed. Malden, MA: Blackwell Pub, 2006.
[66]
J. Belsky, D. L. Vandell, M. Burchinal, K. A. Clarke-Stewart, K. McCartney, and M. T. Owen, ‘Are There Long-Term Effects of Early Child Care?’, Child Development, vol. 78, no. 2, pp. 681–701, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01021.x.
[67]
A. Clarke-Stewart and R. D. Parke, ‘Family: early and enduring influences’, in Social development / Alison Clarke-Stewart, Ross D. Parke., pp. 188–226 [Online]. Available: https://contentstore.cla.co.uk/secure/link?id=14923608-7936-e711-80c9-005056af4099
[68]
R. Plomin, ‘The interplay between genes and environment’, in Behavioral genetics, 5th ed., New York: Worth, 2008, pp. 305–333.