[1]
Adams, J., K. 1957. Laboratory studies of behaviour without awareness. Psychological Bulletin. 54, 5 (1957), 383–405.
[2]
Adams, J.K. 1957. Laboratory Studies of Behaviour Without Awareness. Psychological Bulletin. 54, 5 (1957).
[3]
Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures: 1993. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8505704?report=abstract.
[4]
Bahrami, B. et al. 27AD. Optimally interacting minds. Science. 329, 5995 (27AD), 1081–1085.
[5]
Beck, D.M. et al. 2001. Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness. Nature Neuroscience. 4, 6 (2001), 645–650.
[6]
Campbell-Meiklejohn, D.K. et al. 2010. How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects. Current Biology. 20, 13 (Jul. 2010), 1165–1170. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.055.
[7]
Carmel, D. et al. 5AD. Conscious Awareness of Flicker in Humans Involves Frontal and Parietal Cortex. Current Biology. 16, 9 (5AD), 907–911.
[8]
Carruthers, P. 1AD. Meta-cognition in Animals: A Skeptical Look. Journal compilation. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
[9]
Cartwright-Finch, U. and Lavie, N. 1AD. The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition - Journal - Elsevier. (1AD).
[10]
Chaudhuri, A. 3AD. Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention. Nature. 344, (3AD).
[11]
Cialdini, R.B. and Goldstein, N.J. Social Influence: Compliance and Conformity. Annual Review of Psychology. 55, 591–621.
[12]
Daniel J. Simons and Michael S. Ambinder Change Blindness: Theory and Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 14, 1, 44–48.
[13]
Education Portal Self-Comparison Theory: Upward vs. Downward Social Comparison.
[14]
Festinger, L. 5AD. A Theory of Social Comparison Process. Human Relations. 7, 2 (5AD), 117–140.
[15]
Fleming, S.M. et al. 19AD. Metacognition: computation, biology and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 1594 (19AD), 1280–1286.
[16]
Fleming, S.M. et al. 19AD. Metacognition: computation, biology and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 1594 (19AD), 1280–1286.
[17]
de Fockert, J.W. and Bremner, A.J. 2011. Release of inattentional blindness by high working memory load: Elucidating the relationship between working memory and selective attention. Cognition. 121, 3 (Dec. 2011), 400–408.
[18]
Greenberg, D.L. 2007. Comment on ‘Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State’. Science. 315, 5816 (Mar. 2007), 1221b–1221b. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135284.
[19]
Griffin, D.R. 2001. Animals know more than we used to think. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 98, 9 (Apr. 2001), 4833–4834. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.091088198.
[20]
Heider, F. Attitudes and Cognitive Organization. The Journal of Psychology. 21, 1, 107–112.
[21]
Hertwig, R. 2012. Tapping into the Wisdom of the Crowd--with Confidence. Science. 336, 6079 (Apr. 2012), 303–304. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1221403.
[22]
Izuma, K. and Adolphs, R. 2013. Social Manipulation of Preference in the Human Brain. Neuron. 78, 3 (May 2013), 563–573. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.023.
[23]
Johnson, D.D.P. and Fowler, J.H. The evolution of overconfidence. Nature. 477, 7364, 317–320.
[24]
Kepecs, A. and Mainen, Z.F. 2012. A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 1594 (May 2012), 1322–1337.
[25]
Konstantinou, N. et al. 2012. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Visual short-term memory load reduces retinotopic cortex response to contrast. 24, 11 (Nov. 2012).
[26]
Konstantinou, N. et al. 2012. Visual Short-term Memory Load Reduces Retinotopic Cortex Response to Contrast. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24, 11 (Nov. 2012), 2199–2210.
[27]
Koriat, A. 2012. When Are Two Heads Better than One and Why? Science. 336, 6079 (Apr. 2012), 360–362. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1216549.
[28]
Kruger, J. and Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of personality and social psychology. 77, 6, 1121–1134.
[29]
Lavie, N. 2005. Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9, 2 (2005), 75–82.
[30]
Lavie, N. Load Theory of Selective Attention and Cognitive Control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 133, 3, 339–354.
[31]
Lavie, N. et al. 2004. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 133, 3 (2004), 339–354.
[32]
Lavie, N. 3AD. The role of perceptual load in visual awareness. Brain Research. 1080, 1 (3AD), 91–100.
[33]
Lavie, N. and de Fockert, J. 8AD. The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 12, 4 (8AD), 669–674.
[34]
Lavie, N. and de Fockert, J. 8AD. The role of working memory in attentional capture. In press in Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[35]
Lavie, N. and de Fockert, J. 8AD. The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 12, 4 (8AD), 669–674.
[36]
Mack, A. and Rock, I. 2000. Inattentional blindness. MIT Press.
[37]
Merikle, P.M. et al. 4AD. Perception without awareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition. 79, 1–2 (4AD), 115–134.
[38]
Moore, C.M. and Egeth, H. 1997. Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance. 23, 2 (1997).
[39]
Most, S.B. et al. 2001. How not to be Seen: The Contribution of Similarity and Selective Ignoring to Sustained Inattentional Blindness. PubMed.gov. 12, 1 (Jan. 2001), 9–17. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00303.
[40]
Most, S.B. et al. 1AD. How not to be seen: the contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. PubMed.gov. 12, 1 (1AD), 9–17.
[41]
Murphy, S.T. Affect, Cognition, and Awareness: Affective Priming With Optimal and Suboptimal Stimulus Exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64, 5, 723–739.
[42]
Nachev, P. and Husain, M. 2007. Comment on ‘Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State’. Science. 315, 5816 (Mar. 2007), 1221a–1221a. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135096.
[43]
Narayanan, N.S. et al. Common medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nature Neuroscience. 16, 12, 1888–1895.
[44]
O’Regan, J.K. et al. 3AD. Change-blindness as a result of ‘mudsplashes’. Nature. 398, 6722 (3AD).
[45]
Owen, A.M. 2006. Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State. Science. 313, 5792 (Sep. 2006), 1402–1402. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130197.
[46]
PubMed.gov: 1AD. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17129748.
[47]
PubMed.gov: 8AD. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=cohen nakayama cavanagh %5Bau%5D.
[48]
PubMed.gov: 3AD. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17346967.
[49]
PubMed.gov: 3AD. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18484818.
[50]
Raft Kunst-Wilson, W. and Zajonc, R.B. 2AD. Affective Discrimination of Stimuli that cannot be Recognized. Science. 207, 4430 (2AD), 557–558.
[51]
Rees, G. et al. 4AD. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3, 4 (4AD), 261–270.
[52]
Rees, G. and Lavie, N. 1AD. What can functional imaging reveal about the role of attention in visual awareness? Neuropsychologia. 39, 2 (1AD), 1343–1353.
[53]
Rensink, R.A. et al. 1997. To See or not to See: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes. Psychological Science. 8, 5 (Sep. 1997), 368–373.
[54]
Sapolsky, R.S. 29AD. The Influence of Social Hierarchy on Primate Health. Science. 308, (29AD), 648–652.
[55]
Schnyer, D.M. et al. 2004. A role for right medial prefrontal cortex in accurate feeling-of-knowing judgments: evidence from patients with lesions to frontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 42, 7 (2004), 957–966.
[56]
Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic  events. Perception. 28, 9 (1999), 1059–1074. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1068/p2952.
[57]
Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. PubMed.gov. 28, 9 (1999), 1059–1074.
[58]
Simons, D.J. and Levin, D.T. 1998. Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 5, 4 (Dec. 1998), 644–649.
[59]
Smith, J.D. et al. 2012. The highs and lows of theoretical interpretation in animal-metacognition research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 1594 (May 2012), 1297–1309.
[60]
Summerfield, C. and Yeung, N. Oh, rats! Post-error behavioral adjustment in creatures great and small. Nature Neuroscience. 16, 1715–1716.
[61]
Terrace, H.S. and Son, L.K. 2009. Comparative metacognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 19, 1 (Feb. 2009), 67–74. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.06.004.
[62]
Tsal, Y. and Kolbet, L. 2AD. Disambiguating ambiguous figures by selective attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 37, 1 (2AD), 25–37.
[63]
Ullsperger, M. et al. Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 18, 5, 259–267.
[64]
Wright, N.D. et al. 7AD. Testosterone disrupts human collaboration by increasing egocentric choices. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society: Biological Sciences. 279, 1736 (7AD), 2275–2280.
[65]
Yeung, N. and Summerfield, C. 2012. Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoring. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367, 1594 (May 2012), 1310–1321.
[66]
Yokoyama, T. et al. 2014. Perception of Direct Gaze Does Not Require Focus of Attention. Scientific Reports. 4, (Jan. 2014). DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/srep03858.
[67]
Zaki, J. and Ochsner, K. 2011. Reintegrating the Study of Accuracy Into Social Cognition Research. Psychological Inquiry. 22, 3 (Aug. 2011), 159–182. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2011.551743.